Autism ADHD overlap?
ASPartOfMe
Veteran
Joined: 25 Aug 2013
Age: 67
Gender: Male
Posts: 36,541
Location: Long Island, New York
The connections could run deep. One team in the Netherlands has proposed that autism and ADHD are different manifestations of a single condition with a range of subtypes, each having a distinct time of onset, mix of traits and progression. In this view, ADHD can occur without signs of autism, but autism always occurs with features of ADHD or other conditions, notes the team’s leader Jan Buitelaar, professor of child and adolescent psychiatry at Radboud University in Nijmegen, the Netherlands.
However tempting the ‘one-condition’ idea may seem, the evidence is far from conclusive. A growing number of genetic studies support the notion of at least some shared causation between autism and ADHD. But imaging studies comparing brain structures and connectivity have yielded a confusing mix of similarities and differences. And some behavioral research has highlighted the possibility that outwardly similar features mask distinct underlying mechanisms. Inattention in a person with autism, for example, might result from sensory overload, and apparent social problems in someone with ADHD may reflect impulsivity.
“Are we looking at one condition that’s on a continuum, or two distinct conditions? I think we don’t know the answer to that question,” says Geraldine Dawson, director of the Duke Center for Autism and Brain Development. “There hasn’t really been enough systematic research.”
The idea that autism and ADHD are intrinsically entwined stems not just from their frequent co-occurrence, but from observations that they share behavioral features. The core diagnostic criteria for the conditions remain distinct in the DSM-5: social communication impairments plus restricted and repetitive behaviors for autism; and inattention or hyperactivity and impulsivity, or a combination, for ADHD. But both conditions can involve delays in language, heightened sensory responses, defiant behavior, problems with regulating emotions and difficulty with planning and with inhibiting behavior. Both also appear in childhood and are more often diagnosed in boys.
Some of the strongest support for shared roots comes from studies of families and twins, which indicate that relatives of people with either condition have an increased risk of having both. For instance, firstborn children of women with ADHD face a sixfold risk of also having ADHD and more than double the risk of having autism compared with the general population, according to a 2014 study.
Last year, another team considered the risks in reverse. Reviewing medical data from nearly 2 million people born in Sweden, they found that individuals with autism and their extended family members have an elevated risk of ADHD. As would be expected for highly heritable conditions, ADHD risk is highest among identical twins of people with autism, and is elevated even in cousins. The heightened vulnerability in cousins strengthens the idea of a genetic connection between the two conditions — because cousins are typically more genetically alike than nonrelatives but less likely than twins to have had the same environmental exposures early in development.
These results indicate that at least some shared genetic risk factors underpin both conditions. But the picture becomes murkier at the level of individual genes.
Autism and ADHD are both thought to involve multiple genes, many of which may individually exert small effects. Also, both conditions are heterogeneous, meaning the specific genes involved can differ from person to person. Adding to the complexity, the nature of the genetic influences can also vary. Some risk factors are so-called common variants — versions of genes that appear in more than 1 percent of the population. Unpublished research posted on the preprint server bioRxiv in November is among the first to find evidence of an overlap in common variants between people with autism and those with ADHD. Other risk variants are rare and tend to occur spontaneously, or may be inherited. Common and rare variants can both involve anything from single DNA letters to larger chromosomal segments encompassing multiple genes.
Genetic studies of both common and rare variants are beginning to show that risk for ADHD and [autism] is at least partly shared,” says Joanna Martin, a researcher at Cardiff University in Wales, who studies the genetic risk of neurodevelopmental conditions. Still, scientists have yet to make a strong case implicating any particular genetic variant in both autism and ADHD. “Studies so far have been underpowered to conclusively identify specific risk variants that are shared,” Martin says.
One of the latest discoveries is that a certain kind of mutation, called a truncating mutation, is more common in people who have autism, ADHD or both than it is in controls. In these rare mutations, a change in a single DNA base leads to an incomplete, and potentially malfunctioning, protein. A 2017 study examined sequences from the coding genomes of about 8,000 infants with autism, ADHD or both and more than 5,000 typical infants. A statistical analysis implied that many of the mutations occur in the same genes in both autism and ADHD, although it did not specify which ones.
For all the buzz around the ‘shared-origins’ idea, the quest to find common roots could fall short. Evidence drawn from genetics, brain imaging and behavioral research all give reason to question whether autism and ADHD are related below the surface.
One genetic study last year focused on people with ‘sporadic’ ADHD, who have no relatives with the condition. The team first sequenced the coding genomes of 11 children and their parents, and found six noninherited mutations in 4 of the children. They then looked at whether the same 11 children, plus 117 others with sporadic ADHD, carry mutations in the coding regions of 26 genes previously tied to autism or intellectual disability. A mutation in only one of these genes appeared in a child with ADHD. The result “does not rule out some overlap but suggests that the origins may largely differ,” says lead investigator Gail Jarvik, head of the medical genetics division at the University of Washington in Seattle.
The case for shared causation is also difficult to nail down on anatomical grounds. Imaging studies paint a hazy picture of whether shared brain features contribute to autism and ADHD.
In 2016, researchers reviewed 23 years of structural brain-imaging studies in people with autism, ADHD or both. The studies examined the volume of different brain regions and assessed the integrity of the wiring in various areas. “Based on our review, we found both shared and different brain alterations in [autism] and ADHD,” says Andrew Michael of the Autism and Developmental Medicine Institute at Geisinger, based in Pennsylvania.
For instance, the team noted that, in general, people with one or both conditions have less robust wiring in the corpus callosum, which joins the brain’s two hemispheres, and in the cerebellum, which is involved in motor control and cognition. Both structures also tend to be smaller than usual in individuals with these conditions. At the same time, people with autism, but not those with ADHD, tend to have a large amygdala, which plays a role in anxiety, fear and social behaviors. And the total brain volume is also likely to be bigger in people with autism and smaller in those with ADHD, compared with controls.
Buitelaar and his colleagues have also reviewed brain imaging studies of people with autism or ADHD. They, too, saw a mix of findings, which they acknowledge are difficult to interpret. In keeping with their ‘one-condition’ hypothesis, though, they concluded that brain maturation in both conditions proceeds differently or is delayed compared with typical individuals. They hypothesize that distinct patterns of thinning in certain brain regions might help them to distinguish subtypes of their proposed autism-ADHD spectrum.
Imaging data aside, yet another fault line runs through the shared-origins idea: Behaviors that look similar in people with autism or ADHD may be unrelated, according to a 2016 review of the early traits of children with autism or ADHD. For instance, clinicians and parents may rate young children with either condition as having ‘negative affect.’ But this surface state often seems to stem largely from distress, shyness, fear or sadness in children with autism, and from anger in those with ADHD. Likewise, the DSM-5 inattention criterion for ADHD — not seeming to “listen when spoken to directly” — is often also seen in children with autism; in them, however, it may derive mainly from a difficulty in processing and attending to social cues.
In trying to determine whether autism and ADHD have common roots, researchers need to look at the motivations that impel a child’s actions and reactions, says Janne Visser, a child psychiatrist in the Netherlands who worked on the study. Studying children from infancy into adolescence, she says, will help to clarify how the features of the conditions unfold and affect the children’s development over time: One condition may lead to the other, or both may originate from a common set of risk factors, or the pathways may even converge over the course of development. “When you look at the first years of life,” she says, “you can expect to get nearer to the origins.”
Another approach researchers are using to explore the relationship between autism and ADHD is focusing on traits, or ‘dimensions,’ rather than attempting to connect genetic or brain features with diagnoses. Looking at overarching diagnoses, some experts say, produces too much noise, given that people with the same diagnosis can differ greatly in the mix and severity of the characteristics they display. “In reality, behavior is very dimensional,” says Dawson, “and symptoms can go from very mild to severe, and you can have few symptoms or many.“
_________________
Professionally Identified and joined WP August 26, 2013
DSM 5: Autism Spectrum Disorder, DSM IV: Aspergers Moderate Severity
“My autism is not a superpower. It also isn’t some kind of god-forsaken, endless fountain of suffering inflicted on my family. It’s just part of who I am as a person”. - Sara Luterman
This is really interesting. I've been interested in reading about the differences/similarities between ADHD and autism and I've been finding them really hard to disentangle. I score in the borderline areas on the AQ and RDOS tests and most of my problems are with executive functioning so I've been wondering whether my issues might be better explained by ADHD.
Really interesting to read about the similarities in brain structuring between the two conditions. I'm going to bookmark this and come back to read it again when I have more time.
I was diagnosed with ADHD in adulthood. I do wish I was diagnosed with that when I was little though, but I suppose back in the 90s kids with ADHD were stereotyped as 'loud, extroverted, class clown', and so instead they focused on my shyness and anxiety and gave me a diagnosis of Asperger's at the age of 8.
But, looking back, I had a lot of attention problems at school. I had trouble listening to the teacher, and I kept relying on the teacher's assistant to help me all the time. On the odd occasion where there wasn't a teacher's assistant in class, I struggled a great deal and hardly got any work done. But, for some reason, this went unnoticed by the teachers.
As a kid I was also amused by things. Like when we had maths and were given shapes or false coins or whatever, all I done was play with them instead of actually using them for the maths work.
I found it hard sitting still in class, and I hardly ever listened to the teacher. I would fidget, play with whatever was in front of me (until the teacher took it away from me), and I liked swinging back on my chair, not for stimming but because I thought it was fun. (I took no notice of the BS story the teachers told us about the kid who fell back on his chair and died).
I used to run a lot in the playground, and I loved playing games that involved running and climbing. I was just full of energy. I hardly ever played on my own, I hated being on my own.
I was very chatty too. I'd talk about mundane things for hours with my grandparents, which they enjoyed. I could also talk for hours with other children, but children prefer to be playing, whereas adults like to sit around and talk. I seemed to be good at social conversations, whether it was with children or adults.
I remember when computers were first used in schools, in 1998, and I hated those classes. Learning computers was like a foreign language to me, and all I did was either talk or just play around while my computer partner did all the work.
I had impulsive urge to annoy other children, which was why sometimes they wouldn't play with me. I used to make silly noises or squeeze my friend's arms. I knew what I was doing, and I knew it was annoying for them, but it was like an impulsive thing I had to do.
Because I was too shy to be hyper in the classroom, I was more hyper out in the playground, and even MORE hyper at home. I used to play with about 3 different things at once, make a huge mess, only watch half a video before getting bored, and I'd be running around the house noisily. I was good at playing with other children, but I had a short attention span with activities that used up less energy, and that'd cause arguments. Also if there was too many children at my house, sometimes I'd whine and cry for ages just for attention, because I felt that I didn't get the attention I wanted when lots of children were at my house.
Yes, I displayed a lot of ADHD symptoms but I never got diagnosed until 2015.
_________________
Female
But, looking back, I had a lot of attention problems at school. I had trouble listening to the teacher, and I kept relying on the teacher's assistant to help me all the time. On the odd occasion where there wasn't a teacher's assistant in class, I struggled a great deal and hardly got any work done. But, for some reason, this went unnoticed by the teachers.
As a kid I was also amused by things. Like when we had maths and were given shapes or false coins or whatever, all I done was play with them instead of actually using them for the maths work.
I found it hard sitting still in class, and I hardly ever listened to the teacher. I would fidget, play with whatever was in front of me (until the teacher took it away from me), and I liked swinging back on my chair, not for stimming but because I thought it was fun. (I took no notice of the BS story the teachers told us about the kid who fell back on his chair and died).
I used to run a lot in the playground, and I loved playing games that involved running and climbing. I was just full of energy. I hardly ever played on my own, I hated being on my own.
I was very chatty too. I'd talk about mundane things for hours with my grandparents, which they enjoyed. I could also talk for hours with other children, but children prefer to be playing, whereas adults like to sit around and talk. I seemed to be good at social conversations, whether it was with children or adults.
I remember when computers were first used in schools, in 1998, and I hated those classes. Learning computers was like a foreign language to me, and all I did was either talk or just play around while my computer partner did all the work.
I had impulsive urge to annoy other children, which was why sometimes they wouldn't play with me. I used to make silly noises or squeeze my friend's arms. I knew what I was doing, and I knew it was annoying for them, but it was like an impulsive thing I had to do.
Because I was too shy to be hyper in the classroom, I was more hyper out in the playground, and even MORE hyper at home. I used to play with about 3 different things at once, make a huge mess, only watch half a video before getting bored, and I'd be running around the house noisily. I was good at playing with other children, but I had a short attention span with activities that used up less energy, and that'd cause arguments. Also if there was too many children at my house, sometimes I'd whine and cry for ages just for attention, because I felt that I didn't get the attention I wanted when lots of children were at my house.
Yes, I displayed a lot of ADHD symptoms but I never got diagnosed until 2015.
So much of this is true for me too. I haven't been tested for ADHD at all, I really want to get tested. But as a child the signs were definitely there. My teachers thought I was partially deaf because I rarely listened to them and often didn't respond when they asked me things. I was sent for multiple hearing tests and they always came back showing my hearing is excellent. I frequently forgot homework, letters I needed to take home etc, I was always losing school books etc.... the teachers just got really angry with me. I got bad reports and parents evenings, the teachers told my parents I wasn't trying, I would never amount to anything....etc. My parents even had to go in to discuss my "attitude problem" with the head teacher at one point. It makes me angry really because I really was trying. I didn't want to be a 'bad' child. And I was also shy and anxious rather than loud and extroverted.
I also liked swinging back in my chair! And just fidgiting in general. I also liked to run a lot in the playground, I got in a lot of trouble for climbing stuff too. I also loved long, deep conversations, I was very shy but with people I knew well I was very chatty.
I was good at playing by myself though, I've never wanted a lot of attention. Otherwise pretty much everything you've described applies to me too.
Huh. I definitely need to get tested for ADHD.
Similar Topics | |
---|---|
Living with AuDHD (Autism and ADHD) |
03 Nov 2024, 10:56 am |
Study on Autism/ADHD Seeking Parents of children 6-12 |
23 Dec 2024, 9:17 pm |
Help ADHD |
27 Jan 2025, 9:32 am |
ASD executive (dys)functions vs. ADHD |
28 Nov 2024, 12:09 am |